full transcript

From the Ted Talk by Imogen Malpas: Can we see time? Exploring synesthesia


Unscramble the Blue Letters


adamna Zhu, Translator

Rhonda Jacobs, Reviewer

Okay. Let's start with an experiment. What color is the letter F? Some of you might have checked the silde just like I did to see if there's an F up there. There isn't. Some of you might ivinstlintecy know what color F is. What if I told you that F was the golden yellow coolr of wheat fields in summer? Would you think I'd drunk too much caffenie before ciomng on sgtae? Would you think I was just making this up? Or would you want to argue with me? Would you be thinking F isn't yellow, F is green, or F is pink? Some of you might be on the same page as me already, and if you're not, then allow me to welcome you to the wlrod of synesthesia. Synesthesia is a neurological cniooidtn, sometimes called the cross-wiring of seness, where a stimulation of one sense - touch, taste, sound, sight - causes the experience of another. And the type of synesthesia I've just been talking about, where F's are ylleow or green or pink, is called grapheme-color synesthesia, and it's the most cmmoon kind. For people with this type of synesthesia, graphemes - so leretts or nemrubs - have colors. And whilst these colors might differ between individuals, for the individuals themselves, they reamin the same throughout their life. These are my personal colors, and they don't change. Once you've got it, you've pretty much got it forever. And these are the hallmarks of synesthesia, and these need to be present in order for synesthesia of any kind to be disogenad. The synesthetic experience must be involuntary, so it happens whether you like it or not. And it must be consistent; it remains the same throughout your life. It must be unidirectional, which means that graphemes evoke cloros, but colors don't evoke graphemes. And it must be automatic, so it happens completely without effort. But today I'm not going to be talking about the most common type of synesthesia; that would be far too easy - instead, I'll be talking about synesthesia at its most rare. Some of you might have had an idea of what I was talking about when I asked you what color is F. But what if I had said, What shape is next week? Well, for one percent of you in the audience - that's about 10 lucky people - this should still make pcferet sense because this is what's known as time-space synesthesia. For people with time-space synesthesia, time itself has a form, and this form takes pyhsaicl shape around the person. And sometimes this form looks like a hula hoop or like a rleolr coaster, moving through and around the body. Sometimes it looks more like a halo, encircling the head and moving as the head moves, or not, depending on the person. And these are illustrations by time-space synesthetes themselves. The science wiertr Alison Motluk describes her synesthesia as if she herself was riding a roller coaster, starting off in January before moving down all the way through spring and smmuer, and then coming back up through autumn and winter to fiinsh again in January. And this, as we can see, is very much a vritlleacy oriented synesthesia, running parallel to her body like this. In contrast, the writer Emma ynoemas tklas about a horizontally oriented synesthesia. So, on a typical working day for her, she sits firmly in the present with her laptop whilst to her right, tea steams into her past, and to her left, papers and nteos cover her future. And this is much more of a horizontally orientated synesthesia. But we can see that in both cases, the person remains at the center of the shenisetytc form. Now, you might be thinking this is all so far from the reality of how I experience time. A visible personal calendar that goes with you wherever you go might sound like something that you have on your phone, not something that you have in your brain. But I want to suggest that actually, when we all talk about time, we use something similar - we use mtepoahr. A metaphor works like this: you have an abstract concept - let's call it X; and a concrete concept - let's call it Y. Now, in this case, time is the arctbast concept while space is the crntocee concept. It's much easier for us to get our heads around space than it is around time. Since time is too abstract for the brain to process by itself, it needs to be given a context. And by using data gathered from the adtoiury, visual, or tactile centers of the brian, we're able to judge the distance between and the location of enetvs that hppaen in time, giving these events a spiaatl context. And so time, X, is meappd onto sapce, Y. And by the way, this mapping is so prweufol, so fundamental to cognition that the act of moving in space or even thinking about moving in space alrtes how we perceive time passing. Studies have shown that just imagining mnoivg forward in a queue or taking a train journey dramatically alters how we perceive time in the real world. And this relationship, of course, is reflected in our language and this usneviarl rule that time is space. I bet you've used at least three or probably all of these in the past week. The deadline has been moved forward or pushed back; the time is coming. Everywhere we look, we run into this rule, that time is space. And it's this rule which allows us to create an internal mental timeline. So how many of you have soepkn about something you wished you'd done yeesrtady or something that you want to do tomorrow? You might have used gestures like this to emphasize that you're tnkilag about the past or about the future. All of us in this room, synesthetic or not, have a mtanel timeline, a preference to associate certain aaers of our personal space with the past and other areas of our personal space with the future. And so, I will suggest that this means time-space synesthesia, as in the enextral visual form, isn't actually a discrete entity; instead, it's part of a much longer continuum on which we all sit, where at one end, the relationship between time and space in the brain is visualized as an external form and at the other, it's experienced internally. Think about it. This is the axis on which we usually talk about our time. It runs like this, often through our body, so we say that we face the future and we put the past behind us. And we can track this internal synesthesia with the principles of synesthesia that I mentioned before. So, is it consistent? Yes, this doesn't tend to chngae throughout our lives. Is it involuntary? Yes, we can't help that we make these associations. Is it unidirectional? Yes, we need to map time onto space because it's so abstract, but we don't need to map space onto time. And it's automatic; most of us don't even know that we're doing it. So, congratulations, you all have synesthesia. But you cannot go yet, unfortunately, because this isn't where the sotry ends. Although the relationship between time and space might be universal, the ditlaes of that relationship are hghily mediated by the context and the culture in which our brains develop. Okay, second experiment. Imagine that I give you a piece of paper like this, and imagine that I ask you to draw on that piece of paper a tiniemle rnunnig from your birth to the present day. On what side of the paper would you put your birth? Now, I'm willing to bet that for most of you, your instinctive aewnsr is that you put it on the left, with the rest of your life running across the page to the right. And I'm also willing to bet that for most of you, your first language, the one you grew up speaking, runs from left to right across the page. And my fanil bet is that if I'm wrong about your first laggaune, then I'm also wnrog about the placement of your birth, since studies have swohn that those whose first language runs from right to left, like aabirc, are more likely to put the evnet of their birth on the right of the page. And this means that the mental timeline I was just talking about, running like this, isn't universal at all. It was thought to be for a long time, but it's recently been discovered that different cultures visualize time as running along axes as varied as left to right, right to left, up to down, back to front, even notrh to south and east to west. So here's an example. The ppowrrmauaan are an Indigenous argbainiol community in Australia, and if you ask a Pormpuraawan individual to order photos of a person aging in order from the youngest to the oldset, they will iilnrbavay put the oldest photos of the preosn on the west and the ynsoegut pooths to the east. And this is regardless of where they are in the room or their orientation; they know where west and east is, and they will order the photos in those directions. So for the Pormpuraawan, time itself runs from east to west. And for the Aymara of the Andes, time runs along a similar axis as it does for us, but it's rvreeesd. So the Aymara logic is, well, we've experienced the past - we know it, we've seen it - so it should lie in front, where we can look at it, whilst the future, which ranemis uonkwnn, lies behind us. So for the Aymara, future events sit here, and past events sit here. And this is reflected, as it is for us in eisnlgh, in their language. So the Aymara word for "last year" literally means front year, or side year. But I think the most fascinating example of this variation in timelines comes from the Yupno people of Papua New guniea. And the Yupno are geographically ielstaod by a very steep mountain range, so their access to lots of wtesren toeengiolchs, like clocks and calendars, as well as electricity and domestic animals, is highly limited. And the ynupo express their time according to their landscape. A team of anthropologists living with the Yupno noticed that if you ask a Yupno person about the past, they will gesture downhill towards the muoth of the river, which is so integral to their way of life; and if you ask them about the ftuure, they will gesture uphill towards the peak of the mountain, and the source of their local river. And again, this remains the same wherever they are in the landscape and even how far away they are from their local rveir. So for the Yupno, time itself and the flow of time is represented by the flow of their river. So what impanltcoiis does this have for synesthesia? Western time-space synesthete's forms, as you've seen before, look a lot like clocks or calendars; they usually run in clockwise or calendrical format around a circle. But what might these forms look like for time-space synesthetes gniorwg up among the Yupno or among the Aymara in a world without ascecs to Western technology? Well, the simple answer is we don't know, since synesthesia studies have overwhelmingly focused on the experiences of Western university students like me, but we can look at other types of synesthesia for ceuls. So there's a variation of time-space synesthesia, which is cealld sequence-based synesthesia, and it looks a bit like this. So people with the sequence-based synesthesia, instead of seeing time, they see strings of numbers as having forms and shapes. So this is a typical number form of someone with this type of synesthesia. And in our modern age in the West, the twists and the breaks that we see in these synthetic forms, they always happen in intervals of 10 - so 10, 20, 30, 40 - and this represents the fact that in the West, we all live under a very pervasive decimal culture. Everything is in decimals. But this hasn't always been the case, not even in the UK. So 150 years ago, Victorian statistician fnaircs gtlaon was doing his research, and luckily for us, he was also recording examples of sequence-space synesthesia. And this is a form that he recorded in 1881 from a btsirih person. And at first glance, this form might look pretty similar to the one I just shoewd you, and in fact, it has a long stretch here across the mlddie, which has numbers ordered in decimal order. But what's inrsetitneg here is where the breaks lie. And we can see that the breaks lie on one side at 12, and then at the other side, at 112 and 120, and this reflects the fact that in vicotairn Britain, the dominant numerical system was the duodecimal system. And what I also love about this form is that you can see a perfectly reproduced clock face just here on the right. I think it's great. But the point is that as culture changes, so does sesinsehyta. So it's cealovbicne, even probable, that ppeloe growing up in a culture without Western ienunfcle would display a completely different time-space synesthetic form to those that we see today in our research, perhaps even one that doesn't focus on the body but one that focuses on the ldnsapace, which as far as we know, has never been seen. And the likelihood is that in focusing all of our attention on Western accounts of synesthesia, we're missing out on an extraordinary ragne of cultural and synesthetic variation that is only paralleled by the earodrtirnxay range of humans on this planet. So, beyond being, I think, quite interesting, why is any of this important? Well, getting to grips with this idea of a synesthetic continuum and its amazing variety could help us to better grsap how different areas of the brain dovleep and work together. Research into brain connectivity has helped us understand so much, from sleep to addiction to memory, but we still have a lot further to go. And I think that launching a dedicated eeodixtipn to the furthest lengths of this sihnttyec continuum could help us reveal vital information about the rmnineiag mysteries of cognitive function. For example, synesthesia reveals unexpected connections between different areas of the brain, so it could eailsy be used to help uerntnadsd how cognitive decline works and maybe to even prevent it, or to help patients recover from traumatic brain injury. And moving beyond senccie to society, I believe that truly understanding how cultural difrcfenees can shape this tangible reality that we see around us, even siphnag time itself, can really help us get to grips with the concept of seeing the world from other people's points of view. If we can begin by dispensing with this outdated idea that we have rigid sensory borders and instead, come to accept that we all sit somewhere together yet at separate points on a continuum of experience, then perhaps we can take one step closer towards dispensing with unnecessary social divisions altogether. And maybe that might even mean one step further towards inhabiting a world that is extraordinarily and jfulyloy uoubnnd. Thank you. (Applause) (Cheers)

Open Cloze


______ Zhu, Translator

Rhonda Jacobs, Reviewer

Okay. Let's start with an experiment. What color is the letter F? Some of you might have checked the _____ just like I did to see if there's an F up there. There isn't. Some of you might _____________ know what color F is. What if I told you that F was the golden yellow _____ of wheat fields in summer? Would you think I'd drunk too much ________ before ______ on _____? Would you think I was just making this up? Or would you want to argue with me? Would you be thinking F isn't yellow, F is green, or F is pink? Some of you might be on the same page as me already, and if you're not, then allow me to welcome you to the _____ of synesthesia. Synesthesia is a neurological _________, sometimes called the cross-wiring of ______, where a stimulation of one sense - touch, taste, sound, sight - causes the experience of another. And the type of synesthesia I've just been talking about, where F's are ______ or green or pink, is called grapheme-color synesthesia, and it's the most ______ kind. For people with this type of synesthesia, graphemes - so _______ or _______ - have colors. And whilst these colors might differ between individuals, for the individuals themselves, they ______ the same throughout their life. These are my personal colors, and they don't change. Once you've got it, you've pretty much got it forever. And these are the hallmarks of synesthesia, and these need to be present in order for synesthesia of any kind to be _________. The synesthetic experience must be involuntary, so it happens whether you like it or not. And it must be consistent; it remains the same throughout your life. It must be unidirectional, which means that graphemes evoke ______, but colors don't evoke graphemes. And it must be automatic, so it happens completely without effort. But today I'm not going to be talking about the most common type of synesthesia; that would be far too easy - instead, I'll be talking about synesthesia at its most rare. Some of you might have had an idea of what I was talking about when I asked you what color is F. But what if I had said, What shape is next week? Well, for one percent of you in the audience - that's about 10 lucky people - this should still make _______ sense because this is what's known as time-space synesthesia. For people with time-space synesthesia, time itself has a form, and this form takes ________ shape around the person. And sometimes this form looks like a hula hoop or like a ______ coaster, moving through and around the body. Sometimes it looks more like a halo, encircling the head and moving as the head moves, or not, depending on the person. And these are illustrations by time-space synesthetes themselves. The science ______ Alison Motluk describes her synesthesia as if she herself was riding a roller coaster, starting off in January before moving down all the way through spring and ______, and then coming back up through autumn and winter to ______ again in January. And this, as we can see, is very much a __________ oriented synesthesia, running parallel to her body like this. In contrast, the writer Emma _______ _____ about a horizontally oriented synesthesia. So, on a typical working day for her, she sits firmly in the present with her laptop whilst to her right, tea steams into her past, and to her left, papers and _____ cover her future. And this is much more of a horizontally orientated synesthesia. But we can see that in both cases, the person remains at the center of the ___________ form. Now, you might be thinking this is all so far from the reality of how I experience time. A visible personal calendar that goes with you wherever you go might sound like something that you have on your phone, not something that you have in your brain. But I want to suggest that actually, when we all talk about time, we use something similar - we use ________. A metaphor works like this: you have an abstract concept - let's call it X; and a concrete concept - let's call it Y. Now, in this case, time is the ________ concept while space is the ________ concept. It's much easier for us to get our heads around space than it is around time. Since time is too abstract for the brain to process by itself, it needs to be given a context. And by using data gathered from the ________, visual, or tactile centers of the _____, we're able to judge the distance between and the location of ______ that ______ in time, giving these events a _______ context. And so time, X, is ______ onto _____, Y. And by the way, this mapping is so ________, so fundamental to cognition that the act of moving in space or even thinking about moving in space ______ how we perceive time passing. Studies have shown that just imagining ______ forward in a queue or taking a train journey dramatically alters how we perceive time in the real world. And this relationship, of course, is reflected in our language and this _________ rule that time is space. I bet you've used at least three or probably all of these in the past week. The deadline has been moved forward or pushed back; the time is coming. Everywhere we look, we run into this rule, that time is space. And it's this rule which allows us to create an internal mental timeline. So how many of you have ______ about something you wished you'd done _________ or something that you want to do tomorrow? You might have used gestures like this to emphasize that you're _______ about the past or about the future. All of us in this room, synesthetic or not, have a ______ timeline, a preference to associate certain _____ of our personal space with the past and other areas of our personal space with the future. And so, I will suggest that this means time-space synesthesia, as in the ________ visual form, isn't actually a discrete entity; instead, it's part of a much longer continuum on which we all sit, where at one end, the relationship between time and space in the brain is visualized as an external form and at the other, it's experienced internally. Think about it. This is the axis on which we usually talk about our time. It runs like this, often through our body, so we say that we face the future and we put the past behind us. And we can track this internal synesthesia with the principles of synesthesia that I mentioned before. So, is it consistent? Yes, this doesn't tend to ______ throughout our lives. Is it involuntary? Yes, we can't help that we make these associations. Is it unidirectional? Yes, we need to map time onto space because it's so abstract, but we don't need to map space onto time. And it's automatic; most of us don't even know that we're doing it. So, congratulations, you all have synesthesia. But you cannot go yet, unfortunately, because this isn't where the _____ ends. Although the relationship between time and space might be universal, the _______ of that relationship are ______ mediated by the context and the culture in which our brains develop. Okay, second experiment. Imagine that I give you a piece of paper like this, and imagine that I ask you to draw on that piece of paper a ________ _______ from your birth to the present day. On what side of the paper would you put your birth? Now, I'm willing to bet that for most of you, your instinctive ______ is that you put it on the left, with the rest of your life running across the page to the right. And I'm also willing to bet that for most of you, your first language, the one you grew up speaking, runs from left to right across the page. And my _____ bet is that if I'm wrong about your first ________, then I'm also _____ about the placement of your birth, since studies have _____ that those whose first language runs from right to left, like ______, are more likely to put the _____ of their birth on the right of the page. And this means that the mental timeline I was just talking about, running like this, isn't universal at all. It was thought to be for a long time, but it's recently been discovered that different cultures visualize time as running along axes as varied as left to right, right to left, up to down, back to front, even _____ to south and east to west. So here's an example. The ____________ are an Indigenous __________ community in Australia, and if you ask a Pormpuraawan individual to order photos of a person aging in order from the youngest to the ______, they will __________ put the oldest photos of the ______ on the west and the ________ ______ to the east. And this is regardless of where they are in the room or their orientation; they know where west and east is, and they will order the photos in those directions. So for the Pormpuraawan, time itself runs from east to west. And for the Aymara of the Andes, time runs along a similar axis as it does for us, but it's ________. So the Aymara logic is, well, we've experienced the past - we know it, we've seen it - so it should lie in front, where we can look at it, whilst the future, which _______ _______, lies behind us. So for the Aymara, future events sit here, and past events sit here. And this is reflected, as it is for us in _______, in their language. So the Aymara word for "last year" literally means front year, or side year. But I think the most fascinating example of this variation in timelines comes from the Yupno people of Papua New ______. And the Yupno are geographically ________ by a very steep mountain range, so their access to lots of _______ ____________, like clocks and calendars, as well as electricity and domestic animals, is highly limited. And the _____ express their time according to their landscape. A team of anthropologists living with the Yupno noticed that if you ask a Yupno person about the past, they will gesture downhill towards the _____ of the river, which is so integral to their way of life; and if you ask them about the ______, they will gesture uphill towards the peak of the mountain, and the source of their local river. And again, this remains the same wherever they are in the landscape and even how far away they are from their local _____. So for the Yupno, time itself and the flow of time is represented by the flow of their river. So what ____________ does this have for synesthesia? Western time-space synesthete's forms, as you've seen before, look a lot like clocks or calendars; they usually run in clockwise or calendrical format around a circle. But what might these forms look like for time-space synesthetes _______ up among the Yupno or among the Aymara in a world without ______ to Western technology? Well, the simple answer is we don't know, since synesthesia studies have overwhelmingly focused on the experiences of Western university students like me, but we can look at other types of synesthesia for _____. So there's a variation of time-space synesthesia, which is ______ sequence-based synesthesia, and it looks a bit like this. So people with the sequence-based synesthesia, instead of seeing time, they see strings of numbers as having forms and shapes. So this is a typical number form of someone with this type of synesthesia. And in our modern age in the West, the twists and the breaks that we see in these synthetic forms, they always happen in intervals of 10 - so 10, 20, 30, 40 - and this represents the fact that in the West, we all live under a very pervasive decimal culture. Everything is in decimals. But this hasn't always been the case, not even in the UK. So 150 years ago, Victorian statistician _______ ______ was doing his research, and luckily for us, he was also recording examples of sequence-space synesthesia. And this is a form that he recorded in 1881 from a _______ person. And at first glance, this form might look pretty similar to the one I just ______ you, and in fact, it has a long stretch here across the ______, which has numbers ordered in decimal order. But what's ___________ here is where the breaks lie. And we can see that the breaks lie on one side at 12, and then at the other side, at 112 and 120, and this reflects the fact that in _________ Britain, the dominant numerical system was the duodecimal system. And what I also love about this form is that you can see a perfectly reproduced clock face just here on the right. I think it's great. But the point is that as culture changes, so does ___________. So it's ___________, even probable, that ______ growing up in a culture without Western _________ would display a completely different time-space synesthetic form to those that we see today in our research, perhaps even one that doesn't focus on the body but one that focuses on the _________, which as far as we know, has never been seen. And the likelihood is that in focusing all of our attention on Western accounts of synesthesia, we're missing out on an extraordinary _____ of cultural and synesthetic variation that is only paralleled by the _____________ range of humans on this planet. So, beyond being, I think, quite interesting, why is any of this important? Well, getting to grips with this idea of a synesthetic continuum and its amazing variety could help us to better _____ how different areas of the brain _______ and work together. Research into brain connectivity has helped us understand so much, from sleep to addiction to memory, but we still have a lot further to go. And I think that launching a dedicated __________ to the furthest lengths of this _________ continuum could help us reveal vital information about the _________ mysteries of cognitive function. For example, synesthesia reveals unexpected connections between different areas of the brain, so it could ______ be used to help __________ how cognitive decline works and maybe to even prevent it, or to help patients recover from traumatic brain injury. And moving beyond _______ to society, I believe that truly understanding how cultural ___________ can shape this tangible reality that we see around us, even _______ time itself, can really help us get to grips with the concept of seeing the world from other people's points of view. If we can begin by dispensing with this outdated idea that we have rigid sensory borders and instead, come to accept that we all sit somewhere together yet at separate points on a continuum of experience, then perhaps we can take one step closer towards dispensing with unnecessary social divisions altogether. And maybe that might even mean one step further towards inhabiting a world that is extraordinarily and ________ _______. Thank you. (Applause) (Cheers)

Solution


  1. universal
  2. isolated
  3. remains
  4. details
  5. auditory
  6. understand
  7. range
  8. vertically
  9. summer
  10. expedition
  11. talking
  12. youngest
  13. develop
  14. interesting
  15. abstract
  16. senses
  17. yeomans
  18. landscape
  19. diagnosed
  20. yesterday
  21. influence
  22. writer
  23. metaphor
  24. finish
  25. notes
  26. person
  27. francis
  28. growing
  29. shown
  30. spoken
  31. external
  32. powerful
  33. middle
  34. final
  35. science
  36. mapped
  37. differences
  38. happen
  39. roller
  40. event
  41. perfect
  42. colors
  43. western
  44. slide
  45. oldest
  46. synesthesia
  47. change
  48. timeline
  49. technologies
  50. guinea
  51. arabic
  52. moving
  53. stage
  54. spatial
  55. alters
  56. showed
  57. pormpuraawan
  58. people
  59. mental
  60. yupno
  61. numbers
  62. talks
  63. english
  64. physical
  65. remain
  66. letters
  67. galton
  68. called
  69. photos
  70. wrong
  71. conceivable
  72. unknown
  73. british
  74. common
  75. caffeine
  76. remaining
  77. condition
  78. victorian
  79. world
  80. highly
  81. events
  82. extraordinary
  83. river
  84. reversed
  85. clues
  86. story
  87. access
  88. unbound
  89. synthetic
  90. instinctively
  91. space
  92. language
  93. aboriginal
  94. answer
  95. color
  96. brain
  97. yellow
  98. grasp
  99. future
  100. running
  101. implications
  102. easily
  103. concrete
  104. joyfully
  105. invariably
  106. areas
  107. coming
  108. mouth
  109. synesthetic
  110. north
  111. amanda
  112. shaping

Original Text


Amanda Zhu, Translator

Rhonda Jacobs, Reviewer

Okay. Let's start with an experiment. What color is the letter F? Some of you might have checked the slide just like I did to see if there's an F up there. There isn't. Some of you might instinctively know what color F is. What if I told you that F was the golden yellow color of wheat fields in summer? Would you think I'd drunk too much caffeine before coming on stage? Would you think I was just making this up? Or would you want to argue with me? Would you be thinking F isn't yellow, F is green, or F is pink? Some of you might be on the same page as me already, and if you're not, then allow me to welcome you to the world of synesthesia. Synesthesia is a neurological condition, sometimes called the cross-wiring of senses, where a stimulation of one sense - touch, taste, sound, sight - causes the experience of another. And the type of synesthesia I've just been talking about, where F's are yellow or green or pink, is called grapheme-color synesthesia, and it's the most common kind. For people with this type of synesthesia, graphemes - so letters or numbers - have colors. And whilst these colors might differ between individuals, for the individuals themselves, they remain the same throughout their life. These are my personal colors, and they don't change. Once you've got it, you've pretty much got it forever. And these are the hallmarks of synesthesia, and these need to be present in order for synesthesia of any kind to be diagnosed. The synesthetic experience must be involuntary, so it happens whether you like it or not. And it must be consistent; it remains the same throughout your life. It must be unidirectional, which means that graphemes evoke colors, but colors don't evoke graphemes. And it must be automatic, so it happens completely without effort. But today I'm not going to be talking about the most common type of synesthesia; that would be far too easy - instead, I'll be talking about synesthesia at its most rare. Some of you might have had an idea of what I was talking about when I asked you what color is F. But what if I had said, What shape is next week? Well, for one percent of you in the audience - that's about 10 lucky people - this should still make perfect sense because this is what's known as time-space synesthesia. For people with time-space synesthesia, time itself has a form, and this form takes physical shape around the person. And sometimes this form looks like a hula hoop or like a roller coaster, moving through and around the body. Sometimes it looks more like a halo, encircling the head and moving as the head moves, or not, depending on the person. And these are illustrations by time-space synesthetes themselves. The science writer Alison Motluk describes her synesthesia as if she herself was riding a roller coaster, starting off in January before moving down all the way through spring and summer, and then coming back up through autumn and winter to finish again in January. And this, as we can see, is very much a vertically oriented synesthesia, running parallel to her body like this. In contrast, the writer Emma Yeomans talks about a horizontally oriented synesthesia. So, on a typical working day for her, she sits firmly in the present with her laptop whilst to her right, tea steams into her past, and to her left, papers and notes cover her future. And this is much more of a horizontally orientated synesthesia. But we can see that in both cases, the person remains at the center of the synesthetic form. Now, you might be thinking this is all so far from the reality of how I experience time. A visible personal calendar that goes with you wherever you go might sound like something that you have on your phone, not something that you have in your brain. But I want to suggest that actually, when we all talk about time, we use something similar - we use metaphor. A metaphor works like this: you have an abstract concept - let's call it X; and a concrete concept - let's call it Y. Now, in this case, time is the abstract concept while space is the concrete concept. It's much easier for us to get our heads around space than it is around time. Since time is too abstract for the brain to process by itself, it needs to be given a context. And by using data gathered from the auditory, visual, or tactile centers of the brain, we're able to judge the distance between and the location of events that happen in time, giving these events a spatial context. And so time, X, is mapped onto space, Y. And by the way, this mapping is so powerful, so fundamental to cognition that the act of moving in space or even thinking about moving in space alters how we perceive time passing. Studies have shown that just imagining moving forward in a queue or taking a train journey dramatically alters how we perceive time in the real world. And this relationship, of course, is reflected in our language and this universal rule that time is space. I bet you've used at least three or probably all of these in the past week. The deadline has been moved forward or pushed back; the time is coming. Everywhere we look, we run into this rule, that time is space. And it's this rule which allows us to create an internal mental timeline. So how many of you have spoken about something you wished you'd done yesterday or something that you want to do tomorrow? You might have used gestures like this to emphasize that you're talking about the past or about the future. All of us in this room, synesthetic or not, have a mental timeline, a preference to associate certain areas of our personal space with the past and other areas of our personal space with the future. And so, I will suggest that this means time-space synesthesia, as in the external visual form, isn't actually a discrete entity; instead, it's part of a much longer continuum on which we all sit, where at one end, the relationship between time and space in the brain is visualized as an external form and at the other, it's experienced internally. Think about it. This is the axis on which we usually talk about our time. It runs like this, often through our body, so we say that we face the future and we put the past behind us. And we can track this internal synesthesia with the principles of synesthesia that I mentioned before. So, is it consistent? Yes, this doesn't tend to change throughout our lives. Is it involuntary? Yes, we can't help that we make these associations. Is it unidirectional? Yes, we need to map time onto space because it's so abstract, but we don't need to map space onto time. And it's automatic; most of us don't even know that we're doing it. So, congratulations, you all have synesthesia. But you cannot go yet, unfortunately, because this isn't where the story ends. Although the relationship between time and space might be universal, the details of that relationship are highly mediated by the context and the culture in which our brains develop. Okay, second experiment. Imagine that I give you a piece of paper like this, and imagine that I ask you to draw on that piece of paper a timeline running from your birth to the present day. On what side of the paper would you put your birth? Now, I'm willing to bet that for most of you, your instinctive answer is that you put it on the left, with the rest of your life running across the page to the right. And I'm also willing to bet that for most of you, your first language, the one you grew up speaking, runs from left to right across the page. And my final bet is that if I'm wrong about your first language, then I'm also wrong about the placement of your birth, since studies have shown that those whose first language runs from right to left, like Arabic, are more likely to put the event of their birth on the right of the page. And this means that the mental timeline I was just talking about, running like this, isn't universal at all. It was thought to be for a long time, but it's recently been discovered that different cultures visualize time as running along axes as varied as left to right, right to left, up to down, back to front, even north to south and east to west. So here's an example. The Pormpuraawan are an Indigenous Aboriginal community in Australia, and if you ask a Pormpuraawan individual to order photos of a person aging in order from the youngest to the oldest, they will invariably put the oldest photos of the person on the west and the youngest photos to the east. And this is regardless of where they are in the room or their orientation; they know where west and east is, and they will order the photos in those directions. So for the Pormpuraawan, time itself runs from east to west. And for the Aymara of the Andes, time runs along a similar axis as it does for us, but it's reversed. So the Aymara logic is, well, we've experienced the past - we know it, we've seen it - so it should lie in front, where we can look at it, whilst the future, which remains unknown, lies behind us. So for the Aymara, future events sit here, and past events sit here. And this is reflected, as it is for us in English, in their language. So the Aymara word for "last year" literally means front year, or side year. But I think the most fascinating example of this variation in timelines comes from the Yupno people of Papua New Guinea. And the Yupno are geographically isolated by a very steep mountain range, so their access to lots of Western technologies, like clocks and calendars, as well as electricity and domestic animals, is highly limited. And the Yupno express their time according to their landscape. A team of anthropologists living with the Yupno noticed that if you ask a Yupno person about the past, they will gesture downhill towards the mouth of the river, which is so integral to their way of life; and if you ask them about the future, they will gesture uphill towards the peak of the mountain, and the source of their local river. And again, this remains the same wherever they are in the landscape and even how far away they are from their local river. So for the Yupno, time itself and the flow of time is represented by the flow of their river. So what implications does this have for synesthesia? Western time-space synesthete's forms, as you've seen before, look a lot like clocks or calendars; they usually run in clockwise or calendrical format around a circle. But what might these forms look like for time-space synesthetes growing up among the Yupno or among the Aymara in a world without access to Western technology? Well, the simple answer is we don't know, since synesthesia studies have overwhelmingly focused on the experiences of Western university students like me, but we can look at other types of synesthesia for clues. So there's a variation of time-space synesthesia, which is called sequence-based synesthesia, and it looks a bit like this. So people with the sequence-based synesthesia, instead of seeing time, they see strings of numbers as having forms and shapes. So this is a typical number form of someone with this type of synesthesia. And in our modern age in the West, the twists and the breaks that we see in these synthetic forms, they always happen in intervals of 10 - so 10, 20, 30, 40 - and this represents the fact that in the West, we all live under a very pervasive decimal culture. Everything is in decimals. But this hasn't always been the case, not even in the UK. So 150 years ago, Victorian statistician Francis Galton was doing his research, and luckily for us, he was also recording examples of sequence-space synesthesia. And this is a form that he recorded in 1881 from a British person. And at first glance, this form might look pretty similar to the one I just showed you, and in fact, it has a long stretch here across the middle, which has numbers ordered in decimal order. But what's interesting here is where the breaks lie. And we can see that the breaks lie on one side at 12, and then at the other side, at 112 and 120, and this reflects the fact that in Victorian Britain, the dominant numerical system was the duodecimal system. And what I also love about this form is that you can see a perfectly reproduced clock face just here on the right. I think it's great. But the point is that as culture changes, so does synesthesia. So it's conceivable, even probable, that people growing up in a culture without Western influence would display a completely different time-space synesthetic form to those that we see today in our research, perhaps even one that doesn't focus on the body but one that focuses on the landscape, which as far as we know, has never been seen. And the likelihood is that in focusing all of our attention on Western accounts of synesthesia, we're missing out on an extraordinary range of cultural and synesthetic variation that is only paralleled by the extraordinary range of humans on this planet. So, beyond being, I think, quite interesting, why is any of this important? Well, getting to grips with this idea of a synesthetic continuum and its amazing variety could help us to better grasp how different areas of the brain develop and work together. Research into brain connectivity has helped us understand so much, from sleep to addiction to memory, but we still have a lot further to go. And I think that launching a dedicated expedition to the furthest lengths of this synthetic continuum could help us reveal vital information about the remaining mysteries of cognitive function. For example, synesthesia reveals unexpected connections between different areas of the brain, so it could easily be used to help understand how cognitive decline works and maybe to even prevent it, or to help patients recover from traumatic brain injury. And moving beyond science to society, I believe that truly understanding how cultural differences can shape this tangible reality that we see around us, even shaping time itself, can really help us get to grips with the concept of seeing the world from other people's points of view. If we can begin by dispensing with this outdated idea that we have rigid sensory borders and instead, come to accept that we all sit somewhere together yet at separate points on a continuum of experience, then perhaps we can take one step closer towards dispensing with unnecessary social divisions altogether. And maybe that might even mean one step further towards inhabiting a world that is extraordinarily and joyfully unbound. Thank you. (Applause) (Cheers)

Frequently Occurring Word Combinations


ngrams of length 2

collocation frequency
synesthetic form 2
abstract concept 2
concrete concept 2
perceive time 2
mental timeline 2
personal space 2
events sit 2
local river 2
breaks lie 2
extraordinary range 2



Important Words


  1. aboriginal
  2. abstract
  3. accept
  4. access
  5. accounts
  6. act
  7. addiction
  8. age
  9. aging
  10. alison
  11. alters
  12. altogether
  13. amanda
  14. amazing
  15. andes
  16. animals
  17. answer
  18. anthropologists
  19. applause
  20. arabic
  21. areas
  22. argue
  23. asked
  24. associate
  25. associations
  26. attention
  27. audience
  28. auditory
  29. australia
  30. automatic
  31. autumn
  32. axes
  33. axis
  34. aymara
  35. bet
  36. birth
  37. bit
  38. body
  39. borders
  40. brain
  41. brains
  42. breaks
  43. britain
  44. british
  45. caffeine
  46. calendar
  47. calendars
  48. calendrical
  49. call
  50. called
  51. case
  52. cases
  53. center
  54. centers
  55. change
  56. checked
  57. cheers
  58. circle
  59. clock
  60. clocks
  61. clockwise
  62. closer
  63. clues
  64. coaster
  65. cognition
  66. cognitive
  67. color
  68. colors
  69. coming
  70. common
  71. community
  72. completely
  73. conceivable
  74. concept
  75. concrete
  76. condition
  77. congratulations
  78. connections
  79. connectivity
  80. consistent
  81. context
  82. continuum
  83. contrast
  84. cover
  85. create
  86. cultural
  87. culture
  88. cultures
  89. data
  90. day
  91. deadline
  92. decimal
  93. decimals
  94. decline
  95. dedicated
  96. depending
  97. describes
  98. details
  99. develop
  100. diagnosed
  101. differ
  102. differences
  103. directions
  104. discovered
  105. discrete
  106. dispensing
  107. display
  108. distance
  109. divisions
  110. domestic
  111. dominant
  112. downhill
  113. dramatically
  114. draw
  115. drunk
  116. duodecimal
  117. easier
  118. easily
  119. east
  120. easy
  121. effort
  122. electricity
  123. emma
  124. emphasize
  125. encircling
  126. ends
  127. english
  128. event
  129. events
  130. evoke
  131. examples
  132. expedition
  133. experience
  134. experienced
  135. experiences
  136. experiment
  137. express
  138. external
  139. extraordinarily
  140. extraordinary
  141. face
  142. fact
  143. fascinating
  144. fields
  145. final
  146. finish
  147. firmly
  148. flow
  149. focus
  150. focused
  151. focuses
  152. focusing
  153. form
  154. format
  155. forms
  156. francis
  157. front
  158. function
  159. fundamental
  160. furthest
  161. future
  162. galton
  163. gathered
  164. geographically
  165. gesture
  166. gestures
  167. give
  168. giving
  169. glance
  170. golden
  171. graphemes
  172. grasp
  173. great
  174. green
  175. grew
  176. grips
  177. growing
  178. guinea
  179. hallmarks
  180. halo
  181. happen
  182. head
  183. heads
  184. helped
  185. highly
  186. hoop
  187. horizontally
  188. hula
  189. humans
  190. idea
  191. illustrations
  192. imagine
  193. imagining
  194. implications
  195. important
  196. indigenous
  197. individual
  198. individuals
  199. influence
  200. information
  201. inhabiting
  202. injury
  203. instinctive
  204. instinctively
  205. integral
  206. interesting
  207. internal
  208. internally
  209. intervals
  210. invariably
  211. involuntary
  212. isolated
  213. jacobs
  214. january
  215. journey
  216. joyfully
  217. judge
  218. kind
  219. landscape
  220. language
  221. laptop
  222. launching
  223. left
  224. lengths
  225. letter
  226. letters
  227. lie
  228. lies
  229. life
  230. likelihood
  231. limited
  232. literally
  233. live
  234. lives
  235. living
  236. local
  237. location
  238. logic
  239. long
  240. longer
  241. lot
  242. lots
  243. love
  244. luckily
  245. lucky
  246. making
  247. map
  248. mapped
  249. mapping
  250. means
  251. mediated
  252. memory
  253. mental
  254. mentioned
  255. metaphor
  256. middle
  257. missing
  258. modern
  259. motluk
  260. mountain
  261. mouth
  262. moved
  263. moves
  264. moving
  265. mysteries
  266. neurological
  267. north
  268. notes
  269. noticed
  270. number
  271. numbers
  272. numerical
  273. oldest
  274. order
  275. ordered
  276. orientated
  277. oriented
  278. outdated
  279. overwhelmingly
  280. page
  281. paper
  282. papers
  283. papua
  284. parallel
  285. paralleled
  286. part
  287. passing
  288. patients
  289. peak
  290. people
  291. perceive
  292. percent
  293. perfect
  294. perfectly
  295. person
  296. personal
  297. pervasive
  298. phone
  299. photos
  300. physical
  301. piece
  302. pink
  303. placement
  304. planet
  305. point
  306. points
  307. pormpuraawan
  308. powerful
  309. preference
  310. present
  311. pretty
  312. prevent
  313. principles
  314. probable
  315. process
  316. pushed
  317. put
  318. queue
  319. range
  320. rare
  321. real
  322. reality
  323. recorded
  324. recording
  325. recover
  326. reflected
  327. reflects
  328. relationship
  329. remain
  330. remaining
  331. remains
  332. represented
  333. represents
  334. reproduced
  335. research
  336. rest
  337. reveal
  338. reveals
  339. reversed
  340. reviewer
  341. rhonda
  342. riding
  343. rigid
  344. river
  345. roller
  346. room
  347. rule
  348. run
  349. running
  350. runs
  351. science
  352. sense
  353. senses
  354. sensory
  355. separate
  356. shape
  357. shapes
  358. shaping
  359. showed
  360. shown
  361. side
  362. sight
  363. similar
  364. simple
  365. sit
  366. sits
  367. sleep
  368. slide
  369. social
  370. society
  371. sound
  372. source
  373. south
  374. space
  375. spatial
  376. speaking
  377. spoken
  378. spring
  379. stage
  380. start
  381. starting
  382. statistician
  383. steams
  384. steep
  385. step
  386. stimulation
  387. story
  388. stretch
  389. strings
  390. students
  391. studies
  392. suggest
  393. summer
  394. synesthesia
  395. synesthetes
  396. synesthetic
  397. synthetic
  398. system
  399. tactile
  400. takes
  401. talk
  402. talking
  403. talks
  404. tangible
  405. taste
  406. tea
  407. team
  408. technologies
  409. technology
  410. tend
  411. thinking
  412. thought
  413. time
  414. timeline
  415. timelines
  416. today
  417. told
  418. tomorrow
  419. touch
  420. track
  421. train
  422. translator
  423. traumatic
  424. twists
  425. type
  426. types
  427. typical
  428. uk
  429. unbound
  430. understand
  431. understanding
  432. unexpected
  433. unidirectional
  434. universal
  435. university
  436. unknown
  437. unnecessary
  438. uphill
  439. variation
  440. varied
  441. variety
  442. vertically
  443. victorian
  444. view
  445. visible
  446. visual
  447. visualize
  448. visualized
  449. vital
  450. week
  451. west
  452. western
  453. wheat
  454. winter
  455. wished
  456. word
  457. work
  458. working
  459. works
  460. world
  461. writer
  462. wrong
  463. year
  464. years
  465. yellow
  466. yeomans
  467. yesterday
  468. youngest
  469. yupno
  470. zhu