full transcript
From the Ted Talk by Sam Kean: What happens when you remove the hippocampus?
Unscramble the Blue Letters
On September 1st, 1953, wiaillm Scoville used a hand crnak and a cheap dilrl saw to bore into a young man's skull, cutting away vital pieces of his brain and sucking them out through a metal tube. But this wasn't a scene from a horror film or a gruesome police rpoert. Dr. Scoville was one of the most rnwoneed neurosurgeons of his time, and the young man was Henry Molaison, the famous patient known as "H.M.", whose case provided amazing itginshs into how our brnais work. As a boy, hrney had cracked his skull in an aecncidt and soon began having seizures, blacking out and losing control of bodily functions. After enduring yaers of frequent episodes, and even dropping out of high school, the detaeprse young man had turned to Dr. Scoville, a daredevil known for risky surgeries. Partial lobotomies had been used for decades to treat mental patients besad on the notion that mental functions were strictly localized to corresponding brain aears. Having successfully used them to reduce sizueers in psychotics, svclloie decided to remvoe H.M.'s hippocampus, a part of the limbic system that was associated with emotion but whose fiotncun was unknown. At first glance, the operation had succeeded. H.M.'s seizures virtually disappeared, with no change in personality, and his IQ even improved. But there was one problem: His memory was shot. Besides losing most of his memories from the previous decade, H.M. was unable to form new ones, forgetting what day it was, repeating comments, and even enaitg multiple mleas in a row. When Scoville informed another expert, Wilder pfneleid, of the results, he sent a Ph.D student named Brenda Milner to study H.M. at his parents' home, where he now spent his days doing odd cerhos, and watching cisaslc movies for the first time, over and over. What she discovered through a seeirs of tests and interviews didn't just contribute greatly to the study of memory. It redefined what memory even meant. One of Milner's findings shed light on the ooubivs fact that although H.M. couldn't form new memories, he still retained information long enough from moment to mmonet to finish a sentence or find the bathroom. When Milner gave him a random number, he managed to rmbmeeer it for fifteen minutes by repeating it to himself constantly. But only five minutes later, he forgot the test had even taken pcale. Neuroscientists had though of mmroey as monolithic, all of it eietanlslsy the same and stored throughout the brain. Milner's rsltues were not only the first clue for the now familiar distinction between short-term and long-term memory, but show that each uses different brain regions. We now know that memory formation involves several steps. After immediate sensory data is timrolrepay transcribed by neurons in the cortex, it travels to the hippocampus, where special pretnios work to strengthen the cortical synaptic connections. If the experience was strong enough, or we recall it periodically in the first few days, the hippocampus then transfers the memory back to the cortex for panenmert storage. H.M.'s mind could form the initial impressions, but without a hippocampus to perform this memory consolidation, they eroded, like messages scrawled in sand. But this was not the only memory distinction mlneir found. In a now famous eerpnmexit, she aeksd H.M. to trace a third star in the narrow space between the outlines of two concentric ones while he could only see his paper and pencil through a mirror. Like anyone else performing such an awkward task for the first time, he did hiblrroy. But srrnilgusipy, he improved over repeated trials, even though he had no memory of proviues attempts. His unconscious motor centers remembered what the conscious mind had forgotten. What Milner had discovered was that the declarative memory of names, dates and facts is different from the procedural memory of riding a bicycle or signing your name. And we now know that procedural memory rieles more on the basal ganglia and cerebellum, structures that were ictnat in H.M.'s brain. This distinction between "knowing that" and "knowing how" has underpinned all memory research since. H.M. died at the age of 82 after a mostly peaceful life in a nursing home. Over the years, he had been examined by more than 100 neuroscientists, making his the most studied mind in hortisy. Upon his death, his brian was prsvreeed and scanned before being cut into over 2000 idiviaudnl slices and photographed to form a digital map down to the level of individual neurons, all in a live brodascat watched by 400,000 people. Though H.M. spent most of his life fnottegirg things, he and his contributions to our understanding of memory will be remembered for generations to come.
Open Cloze
On September 1st, 1953, _______ Scoville used a hand _____ and a cheap _____ saw to bore into a young man's skull, cutting away vital pieces of his brain and sucking them out through a metal tube. But this wasn't a scene from a horror film or a gruesome police ______. Dr. Scoville was one of the most ________ neurosurgeons of his time, and the young man was Henry Molaison, the famous patient known as "H.M.", whose case provided amazing ________ into how our ______ work. As a boy, _____ had cracked his skull in an ________ and soon began having seizures, blacking out and losing control of bodily functions. After enduring _____ of frequent episodes, and even dropping out of high school, the _________ young man had turned to Dr. Scoville, a daredevil known for risky surgeries. Partial lobotomies had been used for decades to treat mental patients _____ on the notion that mental functions were strictly localized to corresponding brain _____. Having successfully used them to reduce ________ in psychotics, ________ decided to ______ H.M.'s hippocampus, a part of the limbic system that was associated with emotion but whose ________ was unknown. At first glance, the operation had succeeded. H.M.'s seizures virtually disappeared, with no change in personality, and his IQ even improved. But there was one problem: His memory was shot. Besides losing most of his memories from the previous decade, H.M. was unable to form new ones, forgetting what day it was, repeating comments, and even ______ multiple _____ in a row. When Scoville informed another expert, Wilder ________, of the results, he sent a Ph.D student named Brenda Milner to study H.M. at his parents' home, where he now spent his days doing odd ______, and watching _______ movies for the first time, over and over. What she discovered through a ______ of tests and interviews didn't just contribute greatly to the study of memory. It redefined what memory even meant. One of Milner's findings shed light on the _______ fact that although H.M. couldn't form new memories, he still retained information long enough from moment to ______ to finish a sentence or find the bathroom. When Milner gave him a random number, he managed to ________ it for fifteen minutes by repeating it to himself constantly. But only five minutes later, he forgot the test had even taken _____. Neuroscientists had though of ______ as monolithic, all of it ___________ the same and stored throughout the brain. Milner's _______ were not only the first clue for the now familiar distinction between short-term and long-term memory, but show that each uses different brain regions. We now know that memory formation involves several steps. After immediate sensory data is ___________ transcribed by neurons in the cortex, it travels to the hippocampus, where special ________ work to strengthen the cortical synaptic connections. If the experience was strong enough, or we recall it periodically in the first few days, the hippocampus then transfers the memory back to the cortex for _________ storage. H.M.'s mind could form the initial impressions, but without a hippocampus to perform this memory consolidation, they eroded, like messages scrawled in sand. But this was not the only memory distinction ______ found. In a now famous __________, she _____ H.M. to trace a third star in the narrow space between the outlines of two concentric ones while he could only see his paper and pencil through a mirror. Like anyone else performing such an awkward task for the first time, he did ________. But ____________, he improved over repeated trials, even though he had no memory of ________ attempts. His unconscious motor centers remembered what the conscious mind had forgotten. What Milner had discovered was that the declarative memory of names, dates and facts is different from the procedural memory of riding a bicycle or signing your name. And we now know that procedural memory ______ more on the basal ganglia and cerebellum, structures that were ______ in H.M.'s brain. This distinction between "knowing that" and "knowing how" has underpinned all memory research since. H.M. died at the age of 82 after a mostly peaceful life in a nursing home. Over the years, he had been examined by more than 100 neuroscientists, making his the most studied mind in _______. Upon his death, his _____ was _________ and scanned before being cut into over 2000 __________ slices and photographed to form a digital map down to the level of individual neurons, all in a live _________ watched by 400,000 people. Though H.M. spent most of his life __________ things, he and his contributions to our understanding of memory will be remembered for generations to come.
Solution
- essentially
- series
- temporarily
- eating
- remove
- crank
- horribly
- years
- seizures
- brain
- classic
- henry
- broadcast
- relies
- renowned
- insights
- surprisingly
- results
- chores
- preserved
- moment
- report
- meals
- brains
- asked
- forgetting
- history
- william
- function
- previous
- remember
- memory
- scoville
- accident
- intact
- areas
- milner
- proteins
- experiment
- permanent
- place
- based
- drill
- desperate
- individual
- penfield
- obvious
Original Text
On September 1st, 1953, William Scoville used a hand crank and a cheap drill saw to bore into a young man's skull, cutting away vital pieces of his brain and sucking them out through a metal tube. But this wasn't a scene from a horror film or a gruesome police report. Dr. Scoville was one of the most renowned neurosurgeons of his time, and the young man was Henry Molaison, the famous patient known as "H.M.", whose case provided amazing insights into how our brains work. As a boy, Henry had cracked his skull in an accident and soon began having seizures, blacking out and losing control of bodily functions. After enduring years of frequent episodes, and even dropping out of high school, the desperate young man had turned to Dr. Scoville, a daredevil known for risky surgeries. Partial lobotomies had been used for decades to treat mental patients based on the notion that mental functions were strictly localized to corresponding brain areas. Having successfully used them to reduce seizures in psychotics, Scoville decided to remove H.M.'s hippocampus, a part of the limbic system that was associated with emotion but whose function was unknown. At first glance, the operation had succeeded. H.M.'s seizures virtually disappeared, with no change in personality, and his IQ even improved. But there was one problem: His memory was shot. Besides losing most of his memories from the previous decade, H.M. was unable to form new ones, forgetting what day it was, repeating comments, and even eating multiple meals in a row. When Scoville informed another expert, Wilder Penfield, of the results, he sent a Ph.D student named Brenda Milner to study H.M. at his parents' home, where he now spent his days doing odd chores, and watching classic movies for the first time, over and over. What she discovered through a series of tests and interviews didn't just contribute greatly to the study of memory. It redefined what memory even meant. One of Milner's findings shed light on the obvious fact that although H.M. couldn't form new memories, he still retained information long enough from moment to moment to finish a sentence or find the bathroom. When Milner gave him a random number, he managed to remember it for fifteen minutes by repeating it to himself constantly. But only five minutes later, he forgot the test had even taken place. Neuroscientists had though of memory as monolithic, all of it essentially the same and stored throughout the brain. Milner's results were not only the first clue for the now familiar distinction between short-term and long-term memory, but show that each uses different brain regions. We now know that memory formation involves several steps. After immediate sensory data is temporarily transcribed by neurons in the cortex, it travels to the hippocampus, where special proteins work to strengthen the cortical synaptic connections. If the experience was strong enough, or we recall it periodically in the first few days, the hippocampus then transfers the memory back to the cortex for permanent storage. H.M.'s mind could form the initial impressions, but without a hippocampus to perform this memory consolidation, they eroded, like messages scrawled in sand. But this was not the only memory distinction Milner found. In a now famous experiment, she asked H.M. to trace a third star in the narrow space between the outlines of two concentric ones while he could only see his paper and pencil through a mirror. Like anyone else performing such an awkward task for the first time, he did horribly. But surprisingly, he improved over repeated trials, even though he had no memory of previous attempts. His unconscious motor centers remembered what the conscious mind had forgotten. What Milner had discovered was that the declarative memory of names, dates and facts is different from the procedural memory of riding a bicycle or signing your name. And we now know that procedural memory relies more on the basal ganglia and cerebellum, structures that were intact in H.M.'s brain. This distinction between "knowing that" and "knowing how" has underpinned all memory research since. H.M. died at the age of 82 after a mostly peaceful life in a nursing home. Over the years, he had been examined by more than 100 neuroscientists, making his the most studied mind in history. Upon his death, his brain was preserved and scanned before being cut into over 2000 individual slices and photographed to form a digital map down to the level of individual neurons, all in a live broadcast watched by 400,000 people. Though H.M. spent most of his life forgetting things, he and his contributions to our understanding of memory will be remembered for generations to come.
Frequently Occurring Word Combinations
ngrams of length 2
collocation |
frequency |
young man |
2 |
procedural memory |
2 |
Important Words
- accident
- age
- amazing
- areas
- asked
- attempts
- awkward
- basal
- based
- bathroom
- began
- bicycle
- blacking
- bodily
- bore
- boy
- brain
- brains
- brenda
- broadcast
- case
- centers
- cerebellum
- change
- cheap
- chores
- classic
- clue
- comments
- concentric
- connections
- conscious
- consolidation
- constantly
- contribute
- contributions
- control
- cortex
- cortical
- cracked
- crank
- cut
- cutting
- daredevil
- data
- dates
- day
- days
- death
- decade
- decades
- decided
- declarative
- desperate
- died
- digital
- disappeared
- discovered
- distinction
- dr
- drill
- dropping
- eating
- emotion
- enduring
- episodes
- eroded
- essentially
- examined
- experience
- experiment
- expert
- fact
- facts
- familiar
- famous
- fifteen
- film
- find
- findings
- finish
- forgetting
- forgot
- forgotten
- form
- formation
- frequent
- function
- functions
- ganglia
- gave
- generations
- glance
- greatly
- gruesome
- hand
- henry
- high
- hippocampus
- history
- home
- horribly
- horror
- impressions
- improved
- individual
- information
- informed
- initial
- insights
- intact
- interviews
- involves
- iq
- level
- life
- light
- limbic
- live
- lobotomies
- localized
- long
- losing
- making
- man
- managed
- map
- meals
- meant
- memories
- memory
- mental
- messages
- metal
- milner
- mind
- minutes
- mirror
- molaison
- moment
- monolithic
- motor
- movies
- multiple
- named
- names
- narrow
- neurons
- neuroscientists
- neurosurgeons
- notion
- number
- nursing
- obvious
- odd
- operation
- outlines
- paper
- part
- partial
- patient
- patients
- peaceful
- pencil
- penfield
- people
- perform
- performing
- periodically
- permanent
- personality
- ph
- photographed
- pieces
- place
- police
- preserved
- previous
- procedural
- proteins
- psychotics
- random
- recall
- redefined
- reduce
- regions
- relies
- remember
- remembered
- remove
- renowned
- repeated
- repeating
- report
- research
- results
- retained
- riding
- risky
- row
- sand
- scanned
- scene
- school
- scoville
- scrawled
- seizures
- sensory
- sentence
- september
- series
- shed
- shot
- show
- signing
- skull
- slices
- space
- special
- spent
- star
- steps
- storage
- stored
- strengthen
- strictly
- strong
- structures
- student
- studied
- study
- succeeded
- successfully
- sucking
- surgeries
- surprisingly
- synaptic
- system
- task
- temporarily
- test
- tests
- time
- trace
- transcribed
- transfers
- travels
- treat
- trials
- tube
- turned
- unable
- unconscious
- underpinned
- understanding
- unknown
- virtually
- vital
- watched
- watching
- wilder
- william
- work
- years
- young