full transcript
From the Ted Talk by Mark Changizi: Why do we see illusions?
Unscramble the Blue Letters
Why do we see illusions? I'm going to tell you about some of my research, where I provided evidence for a different kind of hypothesis than the one that might be in the book on your coffee stand. Alright, so let's look at one of the illusions here. And this is a stand-in for many, many kinds of iniuoslls that are explained by this hypothesis. I'm just going to walk through it for this particular one. As uusal in these things, these two lines are, in fact, plleaarl, but you perceive them to bow outwards at their centers. At the center where those radial lenis are, it's wider in your visual field than the parts above and below. And this is remarkable, because it's a remarkably simple stimulus. It's just a bunch of sgtharit lines. Why should one of the most complicated objects in the universe be unable to rneedr this idrcbielny simple image? When you want to answer questions like this, you need to ask, well, what might this mean to your brain? And what your brain is going to think this is, is not some lines on a page. Your brain has evolved to handle the kinds of natural stimuli that it encounters in real life. So when does the brain encounter stimuli like this? Well, it seems a bit odd, but in fact, you've been encountering this stimulus all day long. Whenever you move, whenever you move forward, in particular. When you move forward, you get otipc flow, finlowg oudwrtas in your visual field, like when the Enterprise goes into warp. All of these objtecs flow outwards and they leave trails, or blur lines, on your retina. They're activating mini-neurons all in a row. So, this is a version of what happens in real life and this another vreoisn of what happens in real life all the time. In fact, cnsaioottrs know about this. They put these blur lines in their cartoons and it means to your brain: motion. Now, it's not that in real life you see blur lines. The point is that it's the suuitlms at the back of your eye that has these optic bruls in them, and that's what tells your brain that you're moving. When you move forward, your eyes fixate like cameras, like snapshot cameras, it fixates, it fixates, little (Snapshot sound) camera shots, and each time it fixates when you're moving forward, you get all this flowing outwards. So when you take a fixation, you end up with this weird optic blur stuff, and it tells you the direction you're moving. Alright, that's half the story. That's what this stimulus means. It means that your brain thinks, when it's looking at the first image, that you're actually on your way, moving towards the center. It still doesn't explain why you should perceive these straight lines as bowed outwards. To usnetadrnd the rest of the story, you have to understand that our brains are slow. What you would like is that when light hits your eye, then — ping! — idielaetmmy you have a ptroeipcen of what the world is like. But it doesn't work that way. It takes about a tenth of a second for your perception to be created. And a tenth of a second doesn't sound very long, but it's a long time in normal behaviors. If you're moving just at one meter per second, which is fairly slow, then in a ttenh of second, you've moevd 10 crteeeimtns. So if you didn't correct for this delay, then anything that you perceived to be within 10 centimeters of you, by the time you perceived it, you would have bumped into it or just pssaed it. And of course, this is going to be much worse — (Laughter) it's going to be much worse in a situation like this. Your perception is behind. What you want is that your perception should look like this. You want your perceptions at any time T to be of the world at time T. But the only way your brain can do that, is that it has to, instead of generating a perception of the way the world was when light hit your retina, it has to do something fancier. It can't passively respond and create a best guess, it has to create a best geuss about the next moment. What will the wrlod look like in a tenth of a second? Build a perception of that, because by the time your perception of the near future occurs in your brain, the near future will have aiverrd and you'll have a perception of the present, which is what you want. In my research, I provided a lot of evidence — and there's other research areas that have provided edcevnie — that the brain is filled with mechanisms that try to compensate for its slowness. And I've sowhn that huge swahts of illusions are explained by this, this just being one example. But let me finish by saying, how exactly does this explain this particular example? So, the question, really, we have to ask is: how do those two vetaricl lines in that first stimulus, how do they change in the next moment were I moving towards the center, that all those optical lines are suggesting that I'm moving. What happens to them? Well, let's imagine. Imagine you've got a doorway. You've got a doorway. Imagine it's a cathedral doorway, to make it more concrete — it'll be helpful in a second. When you're very far away from it, the sides are perfectly parallel. But now imagine what happens when you get closer. It all flows outwards in your vuiasl field, flowing outwards. But when you're really csole — ianimge the sides of the doorway are here and here, but if you look up at this cathedral doorway and do your fingers like this, the sides of the doorway are going up, like railroad tracks in the sky. What started off as two parallel lines, in fact, bows outwards at eye level, and doesn't go outwards nearly as much above. So in the next moment, you have a sphae that's more like this next picture. The projective geometry — that is, the way the things peorjct, in fact, change in this way in the next moment. So when you have a stimulus like this, well, your brain has no problem, there's just two vertical lines and no cues that there'll be a cghane in the next moment, so just render it as it is. But if you add cues — and this is just one of many kndis of cues that can lead to these kinds of illusions, this very strong optic blur cue — then you're going to pciveree instead exactly how it will appear in the next moment. All of our perceptions are always trying to be about the present, but you have to perceive the future to, in fact, perceive the present. And these illusions are failed poecpnrties of the future, because they're just static images on the page, they're not changing like in real life. And let me just end by showing one illusion here. If I can, I'll quickly show two. This one's fun. If you just fixate at the middle there, and make stabbing monotis with your head, looming towards it like this. Everybody do that. Make short, stabbing motions. Because I've added blur to these optic flow lines, your brian says, "They're probably already moving, that's why they're blurry." When you do it, they should be bursting out in your visual feild fesatr than they should. They shouldn't be moving that much. And a final one I'll just lveae in the background is this. Here are the cues of motion, the kinds of cues that you get on your retina when things are moving. You don't have to do anything — just look at it. risae your hand if things are moving when they shouldn't be. It's weird, right? But what you have now are the cues that, from your brain's point of view, you have the stimulus on your eyes, like, "Oh, these things are movnig." Render a perception of what they'll do in the next moment — they should be moving and they should have shifted. Alright, thank you very much. (Applause)
Open Cloze
Why do we see illusions? I'm going to tell you about some of my research, where I provided evidence for a different kind of hypothesis than the one that might be in the book on your coffee stand. Alright, so let's look at one of the illusions here. And this is a stand-in for many, many kinds of _________ that are explained by this hypothesis. I'm just going to walk through it for this particular one. As _____ in these things, these two lines are, in fact, ________, but you perceive them to bow outwards at their centers. At the center where those radial _____ are, it's wider in your visual field than the parts above and below. And this is remarkable, because it's a remarkably simple stimulus. It's just a bunch of ________ lines. Why should one of the most complicated objects in the universe be unable to ______ this __________ simple image? When you want to answer questions like this, you need to ask, well, what might this mean to your brain? And what your brain is going to think this is, is not some lines on a page. Your brain has evolved to handle the kinds of natural stimuli that it encounters in real life. So when does the brain encounter stimuli like this? Well, it seems a bit odd, but in fact, you've been encountering this stimulus all day long. Whenever you move, whenever you move forward, in particular. When you move forward, you get _____ flow, _______ ________ in your visual field, like when the Enterprise goes into warp. All of these _______ flow outwards and they leave trails, or blur lines, on your retina. They're activating mini-neurons all in a row. So, this is a version of what happens in real life and this another _______ of what happens in real life all the time. In fact, ___________ know about this. They put these blur lines in their cartoons and it means to your brain: motion. Now, it's not that in real life you see blur lines. The point is that it's the ________ at the back of your eye that has these optic _____ in them, and that's what tells your brain that you're moving. When you move forward, your eyes fixate like cameras, like snapshot cameras, it fixates, it fixates, little (Snapshot sound) camera shots, and each time it fixates when you're moving forward, you get all this flowing outwards. So when you take a fixation, you end up with this weird optic blur stuff, and it tells you the direction you're moving. Alright, that's half the story. That's what this stimulus means. It means that your brain thinks, when it's looking at the first image, that you're actually on your way, moving towards the center. It still doesn't explain why you should perceive these straight lines as bowed outwards. To __________ the rest of the story, you have to understand that our brains are slow. What you would like is that when light hits your eye, then — ping! — ___________ you have a __________ of what the world is like. But it doesn't work that way. It takes about a tenth of a second for your perception to be created. And a tenth of a second doesn't sound very long, but it's a long time in normal behaviors. If you're moving just at one meter per second, which is fairly slow, then in a _____ of second, you've _____ 10 ___________. So if you didn't correct for this delay, then anything that you perceived to be within 10 centimeters of you, by the time you perceived it, you would have bumped into it or just ______ it. And of course, this is going to be much worse — (Laughter) it's going to be much worse in a situation like this. Your perception is behind. What you want is that your perception should look like this. You want your perceptions at any time T to be of the world at time T. But the only way your brain can do that, is that it has to, instead of generating a perception of the way the world was when light hit your retina, it has to do something fancier. It can't passively respond and create a best guess, it has to create a best _____ about the next moment. What will the _____ look like in a tenth of a second? Build a perception of that, because by the time your perception of the near future occurs in your brain, the near future will have _______ and you'll have a perception of the present, which is what you want. In my research, I provided a lot of evidence — and there's other research areas that have provided ________ — that the brain is filled with mechanisms that try to compensate for its slowness. And I've _____ that huge ______ of illusions are explained by this, this just being one example. But let me finish by saying, how exactly does this explain this particular example? So, the question, really, we have to ask is: how do those two ________ lines in that first stimulus, how do they change in the next moment were I moving towards the center, that all those optical lines are suggesting that I'm moving. What happens to them? Well, let's imagine. Imagine you've got a doorway. You've got a doorway. Imagine it's a cathedral doorway, to make it more concrete — it'll be helpful in a second. When you're very far away from it, the sides are perfectly parallel. But now imagine what happens when you get closer. It all flows outwards in your ______ field, flowing outwards. But when you're really _____ — _______ the sides of the doorway are here and here, but if you look up at this cathedral doorway and do your fingers like this, the sides of the doorway are going up, like railroad tracks in the sky. What started off as two parallel lines, in fact, bows outwards at eye level, and doesn't go outwards nearly as much above. So in the next moment, you have a _____ that's more like this next picture. The projective geometry — that is, the way the things _______, in fact, change in this way in the next moment. So when you have a stimulus like this, well, your brain has no problem, there's just two vertical lines and no cues that there'll be a ______ in the next moment, so just render it as it is. But if you add cues — and this is just one of many _____ of cues that can lead to these kinds of illusions, this very strong optic blur cue — then you're going to ________ instead exactly how it will appear in the next moment. All of our perceptions are always trying to be about the present, but you have to perceive the future to, in fact, perceive the present. And these illusions are failed ___________ of the future, because they're just static images on the page, they're not changing like in real life. And let me just end by showing one illusion here. If I can, I'll quickly show two. This one's fun. If you just fixate at the middle there, and make stabbing _______ with your head, looming towards it like this. Everybody do that. Make short, stabbing motions. Because I've added blur to these optic flow lines, your _____ says, "They're probably already moving, that's why they're blurry." When you do it, they should be bursting out in your visual _____ ______ than they should. They shouldn't be moving that much. And a final one I'll just _____ in the background is this. Here are the cues of motion, the kinds of cues that you get on your retina when things are moving. You don't have to do anything — just look at it. _____ your hand if things are moving when they shouldn't be. It's weird, right? But what you have now are the cues that, from your brain's point of view, you have the stimulus on your eyes, like, "Oh, these things are ______." Render a perception of what they'll do in the next moment — they should be moving and they should have shifted. Alright, thank you very much. (Applause)
Solution
- guess
- perception
- field
- optic
- parallel
- shown
- straight
- illusions
- tenth
- brain
- immediately
- flowing
- stimulus
- visual
- lines
- centimeters
- kinds
- blurs
- version
- understand
- objects
- motions
- perceive
- evidence
- swaths
- perceptions
- close
- vertical
- moved
- passed
- arrived
- faster
- raise
- incredibly
- world
- usual
- cartoonists
- change
- render
- leave
- moving
- imagine
- shape
- outwards
- project
Original Text
Why do we see illusions? I'm going to tell you about some of my research, where I provided evidence for a different kind of hypothesis than the one that might be in the book on your coffee stand. Alright, so let's look at one of the illusions here. And this is a stand-in for many, many kinds of illusions that are explained by this hypothesis. I'm just going to walk through it for this particular one. As usual in these things, these two lines are, in fact, parallel, but you perceive them to bow outwards at their centers. At the center where those radial lines are, it's wider in your visual field than the parts above and below. And this is remarkable, because it's a remarkably simple stimulus. It's just a bunch of straight lines. Why should one of the most complicated objects in the universe be unable to render this incredibly simple image? When you want to answer questions like this, you need to ask, well, what might this mean to your brain? And what your brain is going to think this is, is not some lines on a page. Your brain has evolved to handle the kinds of natural stimuli that it encounters in real life. So when does the brain encounter stimuli like this? Well, it seems a bit odd, but in fact, you've been encountering this stimulus all day long. Whenever you move, whenever you move forward, in particular. When you move forward, you get optic flow, flowing outwards in your visual field, like when the Enterprise goes into warp. All of these objects flow outwards and they leave trails, or blur lines, on your retina. They're activating mini-neurons all in a row. So, this is a version of what happens in real life and this another version of what happens in real life all the time. In fact, cartoonists know about this. They put these blur lines in their cartoons and it means to your brain: motion. Now, it's not that in real life you see blur lines. The point is that it's the stimulus at the back of your eye that has these optic blurs in them, and that's what tells your brain that you're moving. When you move forward, your eyes fixate like cameras, like snapshot cameras, it fixates, it fixates, little (Snapshot sound) camera shots, and each time it fixates when you're moving forward, you get all this flowing outwards. So when you take a fixation, you end up with this weird optic blur stuff, and it tells you the direction you're moving. Alright, that's half the story. That's what this stimulus means. It means that your brain thinks, when it's looking at the first image, that you're actually on your way, moving towards the center. It still doesn't explain why you should perceive these straight lines as bowed outwards. To understand the rest of the story, you have to understand that our brains are slow. What you would like is that when light hits your eye, then — ping! — immediately you have a perception of what the world is like. But it doesn't work that way. It takes about a tenth of a second for your perception to be created. And a tenth of a second doesn't sound very long, but it's a long time in normal behaviors. If you're moving just at one meter per second, which is fairly slow, then in a tenth of second, you've moved 10 centimeters. So if you didn't correct for this delay, then anything that you perceived to be within 10 centimeters of you, by the time you perceived it, you would have bumped into it or just passed it. And of course, this is going to be much worse — (Laughter) it's going to be much worse in a situation like this. Your perception is behind. What you want is that your perception should look like this. You want your perceptions at any time T to be of the world at time T. But the only way your brain can do that, is that it has to, instead of generating a perception of the way the world was when light hit your retina, it has to do something fancier. It can't passively respond and create a best guess, it has to create a best guess about the next moment. What will the world look like in a tenth of a second? Build a perception of that, because by the time your perception of the near future occurs in your brain, the near future will have arrived and you'll have a perception of the present, which is what you want. In my research, I provided a lot of evidence — and there's other research areas that have provided evidence — that the brain is filled with mechanisms that try to compensate for its slowness. And I've shown that huge swaths of illusions are explained by this, this just being one example. But let me finish by saying, how exactly does this explain this particular example? So, the question, really, we have to ask is: how do those two vertical lines in that first stimulus, how do they change in the next moment were I moving towards the center, that all those optical lines are suggesting that I'm moving. What happens to them? Well, let's imagine. Imagine you've got a doorway. You've got a doorway. Imagine it's a cathedral doorway, to make it more concrete — it'll be helpful in a second. When you're very far away from it, the sides are perfectly parallel. But now imagine what happens when you get closer. It all flows outwards in your visual field, flowing outwards. But when you're really close — imagine the sides of the doorway are here and here, but if you look up at this cathedral doorway and do your fingers like this, the sides of the doorway are going up, like railroad tracks in the sky. What started off as two parallel lines, in fact, bows outwards at eye level, and doesn't go outwards nearly as much above. So in the next moment, you have a shape that's more like this next picture. The projective geometry — that is, the way the things project, in fact, change in this way in the next moment. So when you have a stimulus like this, well, your brain has no problem, there's just two vertical lines and no cues that there'll be a change in the next moment, so just render it as it is. But if you add cues — and this is just one of many kinds of cues that can lead to these kinds of illusions, this very strong optic blur cue — then you're going to perceive instead exactly how it will appear in the next moment. All of our perceptions are always trying to be about the present, but you have to perceive the future to, in fact, perceive the present. And these illusions are failed perceptions of the future, because they're just static images on the page, they're not changing like in real life. And let me just end by showing one illusion here. If I can, I'll quickly show two. This one's fun. If you just fixate at the middle there, and make stabbing motions with your head, looming towards it like this. Everybody do that. Make short, stabbing motions. Because I've added blur to these optic flow lines, your brain says, "They're probably already moving, that's why they're blurry." When you do it, they should be bursting out in your visual field faster than they should. They shouldn't be moving that much. And a final one I'll just leave in the background is this. Here are the cues of motion, the kinds of cues that you get on your retina when things are moving. You don't have to do anything — just look at it. Raise your hand if things are moving when they shouldn't be. It's weird, right? But what you have now are the cues that, from your brain's point of view, you have the stimulus on your eyes, like, "Oh, these things are moving." Render a perception of what they'll do in the next moment — they should be moving and they should have shifted. Alright, thank you very much. (Applause)
Frequently Occurring Word Combinations
ngrams of length 2
collocation |
frequency |
real life |
5 |
flowing outwards |
3 |
visual field |
2 |
straight lines |
2 |
blur lines |
2 |
optic blur |
2 |
vertical lines |
2 |
stabbing motions |
2 |
Important Words
- activating
- add
- added
- alright
- answer
- applause
- areas
- arrived
- background
- behaviors
- bit
- blur
- blurry
- blurs
- book
- bow
- bowed
- bows
- brain
- brains
- build
- bumped
- bunch
- bursting
- camera
- cameras
- cartoonists
- cartoons
- cathedral
- center
- centers
- centimeters
- change
- changing
- close
- closer
- coffee
- compensate
- complicated
- concrete
- correct
- create
- created
- cue
- cues
- day
- delay
- direction
- doorway
- encounter
- encountering
- encounters
- enterprise
- evidence
- evolved
- explain
- explained
- eye
- eyes
- fact
- failed
- fancier
- faster
- field
- filled
- final
- fingers
- finish
- fixate
- fixates
- fixation
- flow
- flowing
- flows
- fun
- future
- generating
- geometry
- guess
- hand
- handle
- head
- helpful
- hit
- hits
- huge
- hypothesis
- illusion
- illusions
- image
- images
- imagine
- immediately
- incredibly
- kind
- kinds
- laughter
- lead
- leave
- level
- life
- light
- lines
- long
- looming
- lot
- means
- mechanisms
- meter
- middle
- moment
- motion
- motions
- move
- moved
- moving
- natural
- normal
- objects
- occurs
- odd
- optic
- optical
- outwards
- page
- parallel
- parts
- passed
- passively
- perceive
- perceived
- perception
- perceptions
- perfectly
- picture
- point
- present
- problem
- project
- projective
- put
- question
- questions
- quickly
- radial
- railroad
- raise
- real
- remarkable
- remarkably
- render
- research
- respond
- rest
- retina
- row
- shape
- shifted
- short
- shots
- show
- showing
- shown
- sides
- simple
- situation
- sky
- slow
- slowness
- snapshot
- sound
- stabbing
- stand
- started
- static
- stimuli
- stimulus
- story
- straight
- strong
- stuff
- suggesting
- swaths
- takes
- tells
- tenth
- thinks
- time
- tracks
- trails
- unable
- understand
- universe
- usual
- version
- vertical
- view
- visual
- walk
- warp
- weird
- wider
- work
- world
- worse